Today, most scientists agree that all vertebrate animals — mammals, birds, reptiles, amphibians and fish — are, to varying degrees, sentient.
This capacity to feel encompasses a universe of positive and negative physical and emotional experiences. A recent bill passed in Quebec calls animals ‘sentient beings’ instead of property and includes jail time for cruelty.
Even goldfish don’t escape the proposed rules. Although this perspective wasn’t always popular the collection of evidence we now have makes it impossible to dismiss. Prestigious scientific journals publish essays on joy in rats, grief in elephants and empathy in mice and no one blinks. The Nov. 2019 edition of National Geographic is completely devoted to animals and their minds, what they think, feel and know.
The question scientists explore today is not whether animals have emotions but why animal emotions have evolved as adaptations in numerous species. They seem to serve as a social glue to bond animals with each another and somehow regulate a varied range of social encounters among friends and foes.
Although we cannot know for certain what an animal is feeling (for that matter, we cannot know for certain what another human is feeling), we can observe changes in behaviour and physiology and infer emotions. We often hear of animals experiencing or showing signs of fear or anxiety during thunderstorms for example, or after being separated from an owner, or grief after the loss of a loved one be it a pet companion or bonded human.
It is important to let your Veterinarian know of any behaviour changes as they may be symptoms of what your pet is feeling emotionally or physically.
What dogs are teaching us about animal cognition?
Because they are so expressive and responsive to us, domestic dogs have become the darlings of research on animal cognition and emotion.
At the Clever Dog Lab at the University of Vienna, you can watch a dog select symbols on a computer screen by touching it with his nose. In another room, dogs place their heads on a chin-rest to watch images projected on a computer screen. These are not purpose-bred “lab dogs,” but happy pets recruited for the studies. The chin-rest apparatus has been used to show that dogs, like us, glance first to the left side of a human face, where our bilateral brains display more emotion. Thus, dogs get a quick read of our moods and intentions. This all happens in a fraction of a second, and like us, dogs probably are unaware they are doing it. The wag of a dog’s tail also contains subtle cues to their moods. Led by Giorgio Vallortigara of the University of Trento, Italian researchers found that dogs remained relaxed when they saw films of dogs whose tails were wagging predominantly to the right, but they became anxious if the tail wagged more to the left.
At Emory University, scientists are using positive reinforcement to train pet dogs to remain motionless inside an fMRI (Functional magnetic resonance machine), allowing scientists to monitor brain activity while the dogs react to visual stimuli. Preliminary studies show brain reward centers light up when dogs see a hand signal that is normally followed by something good (a food treat), but not for a neutral hand signal. Similarly, when presented with five scents (self, familiar human, strange human, familiar dog, strange dog), dogs’ brains registered the strongest delight in response to the familiar human. It appears the notion that the dog is “man’s best friend” cuts both ways. We use positive reinforcement as much as possible during your furry family members visit our hospital whether it be in the form of treats or tone of voice we want them to be as comfortable as possible during their time with us.
Perhaps more predictably, baboons also have lasting emotional states. Following the death of an infant, baboon mothers show physiological and behavioural responses that mirror those of bereft women. Glucocorticoid hormones — associated with grief in humans — rise and take a month to subside again, and the bereaved mother monkey seeks therapy by expanding her social network through increased grooming interactions with other baboons.
Body temperature provides yet another window into feelings. Human body temperatures rise when we are nervous or anxious about an approaching event, such as an exam or a competition. A rat, handled by an unfamiliar person, gets warmer by 1° Celsius or more. If the same person returns to handle the rat over successive days, the rat’s thermal response declines, and by about day five, her body temperature stops rising altogether. If, however, a new handler shows up on the sixth day, the rat’s body temperature rises again, indicating that her temperature changes are psychologically based. This so-called emotional fever has also been documented in turtles and lizards.